¿Músicos adolescentes demostraron mejor desempeño de funciones ejecutivas conforme dificultad de tarea?

Autores/as

DOI:

https://doi.org/10.33448/rsd-v10i1.11965

Palabras clave:

Funciones Ejecutivas; Musicalidad; Inhibición.

Resumen

Tocar un instrumento musical puede mejorar el desempeño en otras actividades no relacionadas con la música. La literatura actual sugiere que las funciones ejecutivas (FEs), especialmente el control inhibitorio, son responsables de este fenómeno. La evidencia en el área es mixta. Quizás debido a la interferencia de variables no previstas. La investigación actual se centró en músicos adolescentes con desarrollo neurotípico y utilizó el instrumento de prueba de cinco dígitos (FDT). El FDT es del paradigma de Stroop, que se utiliza a menudo en la investigación que se ocupa de las FEs y la música, y mide la capacidad de control inhibitorio. El FDT, sin embargo, tiene la ventaja de una mayor sensibilidad con respecto a la dificultad (que es incremental) y el curso del tiempo (si se cometen errores en la mitad inicial o final) de la tarea. Pudiendo así tener en cuenta más variables. El estudio fue transversal, hecho con 40 adolescentes (14-17 años) de una escuela privada en Curitiba. De los cuales 20 eran músicos (tocaban un instrumento o más) y 20 eran no músicos. Los resultados mostraron que no existe diferencia entre el desempeño de los dos grupos en las tareas más simples, que utilizan solo procesos automáticos. Sin embargo, en las tareas más complejas, que exigen un control cognitivo más consciente, los músicos adolescentes se desempeñan mejor que los no músicos. Esta variable puede ser importante para explicar la discrepancia en la literatura, así como para orientar futuras investigaciones en el área.

Citas

Alain, C., Moussard, A., Singer, J., Lee, Y., Bidelman, G. M., & Moreno, S. (2019). Music and Visual Art Training Modulate Brain Activity in Older Adults. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13, 182. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00182

Alemán, X., Duryea, S., Guerra, N. G., McEwan, P. J., Muñoz, R., Stampini, M., & Williamson, A. A. (2017). The Effects of Musical Training on Child Development: A Randomized Trial of El Sistema in Venezuela. Prevention Science, 18(7), 865–878. doi: 10.1007/s11121-016-0727-3

American Psychological Association. (2017). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct (2002, amended effective June 1, 2010, and January 1, 2017). http://www.apa.org/ethics/code/index.html

Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. J. (1994). Developments in the concept of working memory. Neuropsychology, 8(4), 485–493. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.8.4.485

Barbaroux, M., Dittinger, E., & Besson, M. (2019). Music training with Démos program positively influences cognitive functions in children from low socio-economic backgrounds. PLOS ONE, 14(5), e0216874. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0216874

Benz, S., Sellaro, R., Hommel, B., & Colzato, L. S. (2016). Music Makes the World Go Round: The Impact of Musical Training on Non-musical Cognitive Functions—A Review. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 2023. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.02023

Bugos, J., & Mostafa, W. (2011). Musical Training Enhances Information Processing Speed. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, (187), 7–18. Recuperado de JSTOR.

Cohen, J. (2013). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. New York, NY: Taylor & Francis.

Corrigall, K., Schellenberg, E. G., & Misura, N. (2013). Music Training, Cognition, and Personality. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 222. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00222

Degé, F., & Kerkovius, K. (2018). The effects of drumming on working memory in older adults. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1423(1), 242–250. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13685

Degé, F., Kubicek, C., & Schwarzer, G. (2011). Music lessons and intelligence: A relation mediated by executive functions. Music Perception, 29(2), 195–201. doi: 10.1525/mp.2011.29.2.195

Diamond, A. (2013). Executive Functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

D’Souza, A. A., Moradzadeh, L., & Wiseheart, M. (2018). Musical training, bilingualism, and executive function: Working memory and inhibitory control. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3(1), 11–11. doi: 10.1186/s41235-018-0095-6

Gomes, S. M. (2012). Benefícios cognitivos do treino musical em jovens (Dissertação de mestrado, Universidade da Beira Interior). http://hdl.handle.net/10400.6/2647

Guo, X., Ohsawa, C., Suzuki, A., & Sekiyama, K. (2018). Improved Digit Span in Children after a 6-Week Intervention of Playing a Musical Instrument: An Exploratory Randomized Controlled Trial. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 2303–2303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02303

Herholz, S. C., & Zatorre, R. J. (2012). Musical Training as a Framework for Brain Plasticity: Behavior, Function, and Structure. Neuron, 76(3), 486–502. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.011

Hudziak, J. J., Albaugh, M. D., Ducharme, S., Karama, S., Spottswood, M., Crehan, E., … Botteron, K. N. (2014). Cortical Thickness Maturation and Duration of Music Training: Health-Promoting Activities Shape Brain Development. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 53(11), 1153-1161.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.06.015

Hunter, L. (2018). Musical Instrument Education and Inhibitory Control in Adolescent Youth (Dissertação de mestrado, Goucher College). http://hdl.handle.net/11603/10995

Jentzsch, I., Mkrtchian, A., & Kansal, N. (2014). Improved effectiveness of performance monitoring in amateur instrumental musicians. Neuropsychologia, 52, 117–124. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.025

Linnavalli, T., Putkinen, V., Lipsanen, J., Huotilainen, M., & Tervaniemi, M. (2018). Music playschool enhances children’s linguistic skills. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 8767. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-27126-5

Mann, H. B., & Whitney, D. R. (1947). On a Test of Whether one of Two Random Variables is Stochastically Larger than the Other. Ann. Math. Statist., 18(1), 50–60. doi: 10.1214/aoms/1177730491

Meier, U. (2010). Nonparametric equivalence testing with respect to the median difference. Pharmaceutical Statistics, 9(2), 142–150. doi: 10.1002/pst.384

Meister, I. G., Krings, T., Foltys, H., Boroojerdi, B., Müller, M., Töpper, R., & Thron, A. (2004). Playing piano in the mind—An fMRI study on music imagery and performance in pianists. Cognitive Brain Research, 19(3), 219–228. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2003.12.005

Moreno, S., & Farzan, F. (2015). Music training and inhibitory control: A multidimensional model. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1337(1), 147–152. doi: 10.1111/nyas.12674

Pereira, A. S., Shitsuka, D. M., Parreira, F. J., & Shitsuka, R. (2018). Metodologia da pesquisa científica. Santa Maria. Ed. UAB/NTE/UFSM. Retirado de: https://repositorio.ufsm.br/bitstream/handle/1/15824/Lic_Computacao_Metodologia-Pesquisa-Cientifica.pdf?sequence=1

Pongan, E., Tillmann, B., Leveque, Y., Trombert, B., Getenet, J. C., Auguste, N., … Rouch, I. (2017). Can Musical or Painting Interventions Improve Chronic Pain, Mood, Quality of Life, and Cognition in Patients with Mild Alzheimer’s Disease? Evidence from a Randomized Controlled Trial. Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease, 60(2), 663–677. doi: 10.3233/JAD-170410

Rocha, V. C. da, & Boggio, P. S. (2013a). A música por uma óptica neurocientífica. Per Musi, 132–140.

Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does Far Transfer Exist? Negative Evidence from Chess, Music, and Working Memory Training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 515–520. doi: 10.1177/0963721417712760

Santos, GLS, Carvalho, ET & Selva, O. (2020). Music in early childhood education as a tool in child's cognitive development. Research, Society and Development, 9(7): 1-14, e460974259 doi: 10.33448/rsd-v9i7.4259

Scheurich, R., Zamm, A., & Palmer, C. (2018). Tapping into Rate Flexibility: Musical Training Facilitates Synchronization Around Spontaneous Production Rates. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 458. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00458

Seinfeld, S., Figueroa, H., Ortiz-Gil, J., & Sanchez-Vives, M. (2013). Effects of music learning and piano practice on cognitive function, mood and quality of life in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 810. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00810

Shapiro, S. S., & Wilk, M. B. (1965). An Analysis of Variance Test for Normality (Complete Samples). Biometrika, 52(3/4), 591–611. doi: 10.2307/2333709

Slater, J., Azem, A., Nicol, T., Swedenborg, B., & Kraus, N. (2017). Variations on the theme of musical expertise: Cognitive and sensory processing in percussionists, vocalists and non-musicians. European Journal of Neuroscience, 45(7), 952–963. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13535

Slevc, L. R., Davey, N. S., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2016). Tuning the mind: Exploring the connections between musical ability and executive functions. Cognition, 152, 199–211. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2016.03.017

Tierney, A. T., Krizman, J., & Kraus, N. (2015). Music training alters the course of adolescent auditory development. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(32), 10062. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1505114112

Wilcoxon, F. (1992). Individual Comparisons by Ranking Methods. In S. Kotz & N. L. Johnson (Orgs.), Breakthroughs in Statistics: Methodology and Distribution (p. 196–202). doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-4380-9_16

Publicado

23/01/2021

Cómo citar

GONÇALVES, A. de O. .; BATISTA, C. L. .; DESTRO, G.; MORAIS, E. A. de . ¿Músicos adolescentes demostraron mejor desempeño de funciones ejecutivas conforme dificultad de tarea?. Research, Society and Development, [S. l.], v. 10, n. 1, p. e45010111965, 2021. DOI: 10.33448/rsd-v10i1.11965. Disponível em: https://rsdjournal.org/index.php/rsd/article/view/11965. Acesso em: 23 nov. 2024.

Número

Sección

Ciencias Humanas y Sociales